Este es un espacio para compartir unas serie de temas sobre las ciencias cognitivas y áreas del saber relacionadas
jueves, 18 de febrero de 2016
Anthropology: Hand of the gods in human civilization
In the modern world, we rely on governments, courts and the
police to deter and punish those who would otherwise undermine social
cooperation. But how did human societies achieve and sustain cooperation
before these institutions existed? One possibility is religion: under
the watchful gaze of supernatural agents, people modify their behaviour
in an effort to avoid the wrath of the gods. In this issue, Purzycki et al.1 (page 327)
report a cross-cultural field-study finding that people are
consistently more willing to give money to strangers of the same
religion if the donor believes in a god that is moralizing (concerned
about good and bad behaviour), knowledgeable (aware of one's thoughts
and actions) and punishing (able to exact harm).
Pioneering anthropologists, such as Émile Durkheim and Bronisław
Malinowski in the early twentieth century, have long argued that
supernatural beliefs offer a powerful way to build materially
cooperative societies. But in the thriving new field of evolutionary
religious studies, researchers are drawing on evolutionary theory to
explore how religious beliefs can bring adaptive advantages — that is,
contribute to an individual's survival or reproductive success. Although
major debates remain2, one theory that has gathered momentum is that a belief in supernatural punishment for violating social norms may be adaptive3(Fig. 1).
Figure 1: Weighing of the heart.
This papyrus manuscript, a detail from the ancient Egyptian 'Book of the Dead' called Papyrus of Ani,
depicts a scene in which the dead Ani's heart is weighed against a
feather, representing Maat, goddess of truth and justice. At the top of
the scene are the great Egyptian gods, ready to pronounce judgment on
whether Ani should be granted entrance to the afterlife or banished to
the underworld.
How could this idea apply to cooperation? Deterring oneself from the
pursuit of self-interest because of the risk of punishment from a
watchful supernatural eye would seem to reduce an individual's
evolutionary fitness, and should thus be eliminated by natural
selection. However, even if such beliefs are false and costly, they may
have generated net benefits: to individuals, by steering them away from
selfish behaviour that risked retaliation in increasingly transparent
and gossiping human societies; and/or to groups, by increasing the
performance of the group as a whole in competition with other groups4, 5.
But
what evidence do we have for such a theory? Empirical evidence that
supernatural beliefs promote cooperation is mounting, but has tended to
rely on qualitative, society-level or proxy measures of beliefs. Study
participants have also typically been university students in developed
nations, thus omitting the small-scale societies most relevant to the
evolutionary problem at hand: how human groups achieved cooperation and
made the transition from small to large societies in the first place.
Perhaps the most important lacuna is that previous studies have not
rigorously addressed whether the beliefs of the recipients of
cooperative acts changes people's generosity towards them.
Purzycki
and colleagues' study addresses many of these issues by using
controlled experimental games among participants from eight small-scale
societies around the world and tying the results to explicit measures of
individuals' beliefs. Participants played a simple but clever game
(designed to subtly reveal preferences), in which they allocated coins
between a distant co-religionist (people who were members of the same
religion, but who lived geographically far away) and either themselves
or a local co-religionist. The researchers found that the more subjects
rated their god as moralistic, knowledgeable and punishing, the more
money they gave to distant strangers adhering to the same religion.
Notably, belief in rewards from the god could not account for the
results — supernatural punishment seemed responsible.
Because the
study is correlational, one worry is that some unexamined variable could
account for the results — perhaps certain people are disposed to both
kindness to strangers and belief in punitive gods, for example. However,
Purzycki et al. show that allocations increased for moralistic
gods that were punishing and knowledgeable, but not for more locally
relevant supernatural agents that were also punishing and knowledgeable.
Hence, general conceptions of supernatural agents cannot alone explain
the results. Rather, it is moralistic, 'big' gods that seem to stimulate
generosity towards distant co-religionists6.
The
authors did not conduct experiments to assess allocations to oneself
versus a local co-religionist, nor experiments involving non-religious
recipients, so we don't know whether local supernatural agents might
promote cooperation between individuals within the local community, as
other work has found7, or whether any kind of god promotes cooperation with strangers of another, or no, religion. Purzycki et al.
focused on cooperation with co-religionists beyond the local community,
and thus the expansion of human society from small to large groups. But
future studies of the role of local gods are needed to improve our
understanding of the evolutionary origins of religion (before there were
big groups or big gods), and of whether and how religion brings
adaptive advantages to individuals8.
It
is worth emphasizing that the subjects in this experiment were not
cooperative with random strangers, only with strangers that shared the
same god. We therefore still face the challenge of understanding the
promotion of cooperation and trust among members of different religions.
Purzycki and colleagues' finding that sharing the same god is key to
cooperation suggests that this may be an even harder nut to crack. In
fact, one of the most compelling explanations for why individuals may
help the group at their own expense is that it aids survival in an
environment of inter-group competition. Whenever the threat of
exploitation or warfare is present, the best protection is larger and
more-cohesive societies, which are better able to deter or defeat
rivals. Religion's positive role in reducing self-interest and promoting
cooperation may therefore reflect the costs of competition as much as
the benefits of generosity9.
Religion
is arguably the most powerful mechanism that societies have found to
bind people together in common purpose. From ancient civilizations, to
the spread of Christianity, to today's Islamist terrorist groups,
religion has motivated not only the subordination of self-interest for
the wider group, but even martyrdom in the name of a god. We are still
grappling to understand, from a scientific perspective, why and under
what circumstances humans sacrifice their own welfare for the benefit of
distant others10.
But there is little doubt about the power of religion to promote
allegiance to one's god and group. Purzycki and colleagues' study offers
the most explicit evidence yet that belief in supernatural punishment
has been instrumental in boosting cooperation in human societies. A
large part of the success of human civilizations may have lain in the
hands of the gods, whether or not they are real.
No hay comentarios:
Publicar un comentario